[1]吴 苗.普通话儿童等级含义的习得[J].湖北工业大学学报,2023,(6):74-79.
 WU Miao.Mandarinspeaking Children’s Acquisition of Scalar Implicatures[J].,2023,(6):74-79.
点击复制

普通话儿童等级含义的习得()
分享到:

《湖北工业大学学报》[ISSN:1003-4684/CN:42-1752/Z]

卷:
期数:
2023年第6期
页码:
74-79
栏目:
出版日期:
2023-12-20

文章信息/Info

Title:
Mandarinspeaking Children’s Acquisition of Scalar Implicatures
文章编号:
1003-4684(2023)06007406
作者:
吴 苗
(湖北工业大学外国语学院,湖北 武汉 430068)
Author(s):
WU Miao
(School of Foreign Languages, Hubei Univ. of Tech., Wuhan 430068, China)
关键词:
等级含义语言习得语义 语用界面学前普通话儿童
Keywords:
scalar implicature language acquisition the interface between semantics and pragmatics preschool Mandarinspeaking children
分类号:
G642
文献标志码:
A
摘要:
为探究儿童计算等级含义的内在机制,从而找出其计算等级含义困难的真正原因,采用组内设计,用图片验证任务做了两个实验.实验一考察学前普通话儿童在不提供强词项“所有”的情况下能否计算“一些”所产生的等级含义;实验二考察学前普通话儿童在提供强词项“所有”的情况下能否计算“一些”所产生的等级含义.实验一发现儿童和成人对测试句的接受率分别为83%和20%.实验二发现儿童对测试句的接受率降至67%,成人降为15%.该实验结果表明语境中的强词项有利于帮助儿童计算等级含义.因此,儿童有限的词库更有可能是导致其计算等级含义困难的原因.此外,儿童与成人计算等级含义的差异支持连续性假设的观点.
Abstract:
Previous studies have demonstrated that unlike adults, young children have difficulty computing scalar implicatures. Two distinct accounts have been proposed for this phenomenon. On one account, as computing scalar impliactures involves three complicated steps, including lexical retrieval, lexical replacement and negation of the statement with the stronger alternative, young children’s limited working memory is the source of their failure to compute scalar implicatures. On an alternative account, children’s restricted lexicon causes their inability to implement lexical replacement, which results in their failure of computing scalar implicatures. Against this background, the present study attempts to probe the underlying mechanism of preschool Mandarinspeaking children’s acquisition of scalar implicatures. The purpose is to figure out why children often fail to compute scalar implicatures. Using a picture verification task, we conducted two experiments. Experiment 1 investigated whether preschool Mandarinspeaking children can compute scalar implicatures triggered by yixie “some” when the stronger alternative suoyou “all” is not provided in the experimental contexts. Experiment 2 examined whether children can compute scalar implicatures triggered by yixie “some” when the stronger alternative suoyou “all” is explicitly provided in the experimental contexts. The main findings were as follows. In Experiment 1, the child participants accepted the test sentences 83% of the time, and adults did so 20% of the time. In Experiment 2, the children accepted the test sentences 67% of the time, and adults did so 15% of the time. Taken together, the findings suggest that contextually introduced stronger alternatives facilitate young children’s computation of scalar implicatures. Therefore, our findings appear to support the account that the restricted lexicon is the source of young children’s failure of computing scalar implicatures. Moreover, the difference between children’s and adults’ ability of computing scalar implicatures tends to support the Continuity Assumption. 

参考文献/References:

 [1] LEVINSONSC.Pragmatics[M].Cambridge:CamG bridgeUniversityPress,1983. [2] CARSTONR.QuantitymaximsandgeneralizedimpliG cature[J].Lingua,1995(96):213G244 [3] GRICEHP.Logicandconversation[M].Cambridge, MA:HarvardUniversityPress,1975:41G58. [4] HUANG & CRAIN.WhenORisassignedaconjuncG tiveinferenceinchildlanguage[J].LanguageAcquisiG 38卷第                普通话儿童等级含义的习得 77tion,2019,27(01):74G97. [5] CHIERCHIA G,GUASTIM T,GUALMINIA,et al.Semanticandpragmaticcompetenceinchildren’s andadults’comprehensionofor[C].∥ I.Noveck & D. Sperber (eds.), Experimental pragmatics, Hampshire:PalgraveMacmillan,2005:293G300. [6] NOVECKIRA.WhenchildrenaremorelogicalthanaG dults:experimentalinvestigationsofscalarimplicaG tures[J].Cognition,2001(78):165G188. [7] ZHOUP,ROMOLIJ,CRAINS.Children’sknowlG edgeoffreechoiceinferences[C].InProceedingofseG manticsandlinguistictheory (SALT)23,ed.Todd Snider,CornellUniversityIthaca,NY:CLCPublicaG tions,2013:632G651. [8] TIEU L,ROMOLIJ,ZHOU P,etal.Children’s knowledgeoffreechoiceinferencesandscalarimplicaG tures[J].Journalof Semantics,2016,33 (02): 269G298. [9] HORNLR.OnthesemanticpropertiesoflogicalopG eratorsin English[D].Los Angeles :Universityof California,1972. [10]CHIERCHIA G,CRAIN S,GUASTI,M T,etal. ‘some’and‘or’:Astudyontheemergenceoflogical form[C].InProceedingsofthe22ndBostonUniversiG tyconferenceonlanguagedevelopment,editedbyAnG nabelGreenhill, Mary Hughes, Heather Littlefield andHugh Walsh,Somerville,MA:CascadillaPress, 1998:97G109. [11]CHIERCHIAG.Scalarimplicatures,polarityphenomeG na,andthesyntax/pragmaticsinterface[J].Structures andBeyond,2004,(03):39G103. [12]GUASTI M T,CHIERCHIA G,CRAIN S,etal. Whychildrenandadultssometimes (butnotalways) computeimplicatures[J].Language And Cognitive Processes,2005,20(05):667G696. [13]CRAINS,THORNTONR.Investigationsinuniversal grammar.Aguidetoexperimentsontheacquisitionof syntaxandsemantics[M].Cambridge, MA: MIT Press,1998. [14]GRIMSHAW J,ROSENST.KnowledgeandobediG ence:Thedevelopmentalstatusofthebindingtheory [J].LinguisticInquiry,1990(21):187G222. [15]PAUL P MARTY,EMMANUEL CHEMLA.Scalar implicatures:workingmemoryandacomparisonwith only[J].FrontiersinPsychology,2013(04),1G12. [16]CHOMSKY N.Ofmindsandlanguage[J].BiolinguisG tics,2007(01):009G027. [17]TOMASELLO M.OriginsofHumanCommunication [M].Cambridge,MA:MITPress,2008. [18]SAXTON M.ChildLanguage:AcquisitionandDevelG opment[M].London:SagePublications,2010. [19]CHOMSKY N.AspectsoftheTheoryofSyntax[M]. Cambridge,MA:MITPress,1965. [20]PINKERS.LanguageLearnabilityandLanguageDeG velopment[M].Cambridge:HUP,1984. [21]LUST BARBARA.Universalgrammar:Thestrong continuityhypothesisinfirstlanguageacquisition[C]. In William C.RitchieandTejK.Bhatia,eds.,HandG bookofChildLanguageAcquisition.SanDiego:AcaG demicPress,1999:111G155. [22]CLAHSEN HARALD.ConstraintsonparametersetG ting:AgrammaticalanalysisofsomeacquisitionstaG gesinGermanchildlanguage[J].LanguageAcquisiG tion,1990/1991,1(04):361G391. [23]HYAMSNINA.TheunderGspecificationoffunctional categoriesinearlygrammar[C].In HaraldClahsen, ed.,GenerativePerspectivesonLanguageAcquisition: Empirical Findings,Theoretical Considerations and CrosslinguisticComparisons.Amsterdam:JohnBenjaG mins.1996:91G128. [24] CAMERONGFAULKNER T, LIEVEN E V, THEAKSTONA.WhatpartofnodochildrennotunG derstand? A usageGbasedaccountofmultiwordnegaG tion[J].ChildLang.2007(34):251G282. [25]LIEVENE V M,TOMASELLO M.Children’sfirst languageacquisitionfrom a usageGbased perspective [C].In:Robinson,P.,Ellis,N.C.(Eds.),Handbook ofCognitiveLinguisticsandSecondLanguageAcquisiG tion.Rutledge,NewYork,2008:168G196. [26]AMBRIDGEB,LIEVENEV.ChildLanguageAcquiG sition:ContrastingTheoreticalApproaches[M].CamG bridge,MA:CambridgeUniversityPress,2011. [27]MAC WHINNEY BRIAN.Thechildesproject:The database (Vol.2) [M ].Pittsburgh: Psychology Press,2000. [28]陈宁,范莉关于等级含义产生及习得研究的评述[J]. 现代语文,2015(01):13G17. [29]陈冰飞,郭桃梅汉语级差词项一些的语义语用解读[J].外语学刊,2013(06):63G68. [30]胡建华,彭鹭鹭探究儿童句法结构生长的奥秘:走向儿童语言获得双向 生 长 模 式[J].当 代 语 言 学,2022 (03):317G349. [31]吴庄,谭娟汉语儿童语言中的等级含义:一项实验研究[J].外国语,2009(03):69G75. [32]张延飞国外级差含义研究述评[J].现代外语,2012 (01):95G101.

备注/Memo

备注/Memo:
[收稿日期]2022 -10 -01 [第一作者]吴 苗(1999-),女,湖北荆州人,湖北工业大学硕士研究生,研究方向为儿童语言习得.
更新日期/Last Update: 2024-01-10